Next Article in Journal
Grape Pomace as a Renewable Natural Biosource of Value-Added Compounds with Potential Food Industrial Applications
Previous Article in Journal
Oxidative Evolution of Different Model Rosé Wines Affected by Distinct Anthocyanin and Tannin Contents
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Definition of the Sensory and Aesthetic Spaces of Dry White Wines with Aging Ability by Experienced Tasters

by
Marta Esteves
,
Mariana Sequeira
and
Manuel Malfeito-Ferreira
*
LEAF—Linking Landscape, Environment, Agriculture and Food Research Center, Associate Laboratory TERRA, Instituto Superior de Agronomia, Universidade de Lisboa, Tapada da Ajuda, 1349-017 Lisboa, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Beverages 2024, 10(2), 44; https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages10020044
Submission received: 6 May 2024 / Revised: 31 May 2024 / Accepted: 5 June 2024 / Published: 6 June 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Wine, Spirits and Oenological Products)

Abstract

:
The popular appreciation of dry white wines is most frequently directed to young wines. However, present consumption trends comprise the valorisation of aged dry white wines. Therefore, the present work was aimed at the sensory analysis of aged dry white wines to define their sensory space and to understand which factors drive their quality evaluation by experienced tasters (critics, oenologists and students). Individuals were asked to evaluate several synthetic and aesthetic attributes and to characterise the analytic sensory profile through a Check-All-That-Apply (CATA) methodology. The quality evaluations were differently correlated with wine synthetic parameters according to the taster group. For both critics and oenologists, overall quality scores were driven by persistence and complexity. Moreover, quality was also highly correlated with power for critics and with balance for oenologists. Quality scores were highly correlated with wine browning (absorbance at 420 nm) for critics. The tasting panel showed a homogeneous analytic description of aroma, taste and mouthfeel consistent with wine age. The different ages could be associated with a continuous sensory space characterised by a decreasing perception of freshness and an increase in mature and mellowed descriptors. All wines shared an austere in-mouth perception elicited by their acidity, saltiness, bitterness, smoothness and dryness. The age prediction showed that most tasters failed to guess the aged wines that were more than roughly 13 years old, indicating that tasters were not familiar with the sensory features of white wines from 17 to 46 years old. In conclusion, experienced tasters consistently described the sensory space and recognised the high quality of aged dry white wines. Education programs may use the defined sensory spaces according to aging and to expand the range of quality perception by consumers.

1. Introduction

In wines, the meaning of ageing is concerned with the changes in chemical and aromatic composition that affect their sensory characteristics [1]. The most favourable type of aging in dry white wines is characterised by the loss of fresh fruitiness, and the development of aromatic nuances of honey, beeswax, straw, hay or nuts [2,3,4,5]. The yellow colour of an old white wine tends to vary from straw to amber [3,6]. Deviation from this favourable evolution is illustrated by the so-called atypical aging. In this process, the character of old wines is present in young wines leading to flavours of mothball, soap, rotten eggs, garlic or cooked vegetables, walnut/curry and bruised apple [5,7]. The distinction from wines with proper aging ability is not clear-cut since these aroma descriptors may also be found in developed top-quality wines [8]. Thus, the thin line between proper and atypical aging justifies the examination of the respective distinctive sensory profiles.
The concept of sensory space, defined as the characteristic sensory features of a product shared by a group of individuals [9], appears to be appropriate to characterise wines that are difficult to distinguish. This approach has been applied to the concepts of varietal wines [10,11,12,13,14], regional characterisation [15] and “green” wines [16]. Concerning aging, most research has been applied to red wines’ aging bouquet [17,18]. Thus, the scarcity of research on aged white wines and the present market interest in these styles, justify an adequate definition of their sensory properties, including those with an aesthetic significance, characteristic of fine wines [19].
The approach to develo** sensory conceptual spaces in wines includes three steps: (1) identification of the sensory concept, (2) perceptual evaluation of the sensory space and (3) sensory space description [20]. The first step is performed using a mental descriptive questionnaire [21]. This work aimed to assess and define the sensory space of aged white wines, including the so-called aesthetic attributes characteristic of fine wines. The novelty of this work was mainly in the utilization of a wide variety of grape varieties and vinification techniques and a range of vintages, aimed at defining an overall common sensory space for aged dry white wines.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Wine Samples

Eighteen commercial white wines of different ages and two commercial red and rosé wines, used as distractors, from different Portuguese regions were used in this study (Table 1). The white wines were between 1 and 47 years old, and the distractors were 47 (red) and 15 (rosé) years old. The producers kindly supplied the wine samples. Before sensory analysis, three experienced tasters of the laboratory staff tasted the wines to check occasional flaws.

2.2. Absorbance Determination

Samples were centrifuged at 5000 rpm (10 min) and pipetted into a 1 cm-path quartz cuvette. Absorbance was determined at 420 nm by spectrophotometry (Agilent Cary 60 UV-Vis, Santa Clara, CA, USA), according to Ribéreau-Gayon et al. [22].

2.3. Sensory Analysis

2.3.1. Tasting Panels

The study used three different experienced tasting panels. The first panel (critics) consisted of 9 wine critics and sommeliers (4 women and 5 men), aged between 26 and 72 years (mean 52 years). The second panel (oenologists) consisted of 7 winemakers and oenology scholars (2 women and 5 men) aged between 28 and 61 years (mean 49 years). These participants had more than 5 years of professional experience in wine tasting. The third panel (students) consisted of 14 oenology students (8 women and 6 men) from the second year of the Vitis Vinifera Master of Viticulture and Oenology Engineering (Instituto Superior de Agronomia, ISA), aged 22 to 40 years (mean 27 years). Although without formal training sessions, all participants were familiar with flavour description and professional experience was considered more relevant than training for this study. Three sessions were held at the ISA Microbiology Laboratory. The participants were volunteers and were informed that the tasting would be directed to assessing aged white wines. Critics and oenologists tasted the wines on 14 June 2021. The student session was carried out on 9 July 2021.

2.3.2. Tasting Conditions

The tasting was divided into two parts, with 10 wines each. All samples were kept at a temperature of 19 ± 1 °C, and the tasting room had windows open for natural ventilation. The bottles were opened 30 min before pouring into transparent glasses [23] covered by glass Petri dishes. To limit carry-over effects and memory bias, samples were randomly distributed among tasters according to a Williams Latin square design. The tasting sheet was divided into two parts (Supplementary Figure S1). The first part consisted of a score sheet for synthetic descriptors [24]. The second part corresponded to the taste/mouthfeel and aroma description using a Check-All-That-Apply (CATA) methodology. The attributes chosen for the CATA assessment were selected from a previous online survey [21]. Tasters could choose a maximum of 5 descriptors in each taste/mouthfeel and aroma list. After the CATA, respondents were asked to rate the quality and liking by drawing a line on a 9 cm unstructured scale anchored at both limits. The questionnaire ended with a question about the predicted age of the wine, where tasters indicated the age that they would give to each wine.

2.3.3. Data Analysis

The scores of the synthetic/aesthetic attributes were compared using one-way ANOVA by means of the Chi-square test (χ2) with α = 0.05, since the score distribution was not normal. Therefore, the correlations among the attributes were obtained by using the non-parametric Kendall coefficient (tau-b). Analysis of the CATA was performed qualitatively since the data violated the test assumptions (i.e., no more than 20% of the expected counts were less than five and all the individual expected counts were one or greater). Furthermore, given the small sample size, both χ2 and alternatives tests had too low a statistical power to detect statistically significant differences. Correspondence analysis (CA) was performed based on the contingency tables of the descriptors quoted more than 10% of the time in at least one wine. Two cluster analyses were run, after data standardisation, using the Euclidean distance measure and the Ward.D clustering method, to obtain descriptor clusters (descriptors as rows and wines as columns) or wine clusters (wines as rows and descriptors as columns). Multiple Factorial Analysis (MFA) included the results of quality, colour (Abs 420 nm), age prediction and analytic descriptors. All analyses were performed using the using free statistical software Jamovi, version 1.8, www.jamovi.org (assessed on 30 October 2021).

3. Results and Discussion

3.1. Wine Synthetic and Aesthetic Scores

The comparison of the median scores given by all tasters to the synthetic and aesthetic attributes did not show differences among the wines (Table 2), reflecting the high individual variability in their assessment. The quality evaluation of the 20 tasted wines is shown in Figure 1, illustrating the unevenness of the scores. In certain wines, the scores varied by more than 8 in a maximum scale of 10.
The younger white wines (WLi18 and WDo20) tended to be scored with lower values. The rosé and red distractor wines also tended to have lower classifications (Figure 1). This behaviour indicated that tasters penalised wines that were not consistent with aged white wines by previously being aware of the purpose of the tasting. Interestingly, wine WBe11 (2011 vintage) was also in the lower range of values, probably because its sensory features obtained by the tasting panel were consistent with a younger wine (see Section 3.2.1).
The variability in quality scores by experienced or expert tasters has been documented [25], demonstrating that subjects have a strong idiosyncratic appraisal of aesthetic judgements. Indeed, the comparison between the panels (Figure 2) showed a different quality assessment. Ballester at al. [25] speculated that high correlation among quality judgements may be found among individuals with the same academic background (e.g., winemakers). In the present work, the different backgrounds of critics or the different years of experience (oenologists and students) might explain the differences in quality evaluation, together with a different utilization of the scoring scale.

3.1.1. Effect of Experience on Quality Prediction

To understand more deeply, which factors might underlie the different quality evaluations of all tasted wines, correlations among the synthetic/aesthetic parameters were computed. The scores of all synthetic and aesthetic attributes are listed in Supplementary Table S1. When the responses of all tasters were merged, quality was highly correlated with persistence (linger) and complexity (Table 3). However, each tasting cohort contributed differently to this correlation. Indeed, critics associated quality with linger, power and complexity. Oenologists mostly associated quality with the number of flavours, balance, linger and complexity. The correlations obtained with students were all less strong than those of the more experienced cohorts. Nevertheless, students showed higher correlations with balance and complexity. Overall, these results show that the evaluation of aged white wines is consistent with the definition of fine wines characterised by the valorization of aesthetic attributes such as complexity, persistence or balance [19]. The higher correlations obtained with balance than with harmonious probably reflect a semantic preference for balance. This type of sensation may also be dealt with under the recently described concept of elegance [26], justifying the use of this term instead of harmony or balance in future studies.
Interestingly, quality was highly correlated with liking for critics and oenologists. These results demonstrate that, with experienced tasters, quality and liking are intrinsically related [27] and it is difficult to define if individual preferences are elicited by the aesthetic quality or if preferred wines must share higher aesthetic attributes, as hypothetised by Malfeito-Ferreira [28].

3.1.2. The Influence of Colour on the Quality of White Wines

Colour is a factor that can influence the quality perception when wines are tasted in transparent glasses. As widely recognised [29,30,31], experienced subjects rely on top-down mechanisms [32] to assess wine quality and colour is the first sensory feature to drive their responses. In particular, older white wines, with brownish colour, tend to be regarded as over-developed [33]. In this work, tasters were not asked to rate the colour since the absorbance at 420 nm provided an objective measure of browning. The positive correlations between quality and colour (Abs 420 nm), only for white wines, are depicted in Figure 3.
The tasters were aware that were experiencing aged dry white wines. Therefore, some degree of browning would be expected to influence the results. Indeed, light yellow wines were considered young and tended to be less well rated. Quality median scores lower than 3.5 were only given by critics to the less brownish wines (WMo16, WLi18 and WDo20, see Table 1). Similarly, the highest browning values were not scored lower than 6 for quality by critics (WBa91, WPe00 and WPe08). A lower correlation was obtained with students, indicating that they were not as influenced by colour as critics and oenologists, which was probably explained by the higher level of experience of the professionals [34].

3.2. Definition of the Sensory Space of Aged Dry White Wines

3.2.1. Analytic Descriptions

The wines were subjected to sensory analysis using CATA to obtain an analytical description of aroma, taste and mouthfeel properties. The results of the three tasting cohorts were pooled and the results are presented in two contingency tables (Supplementary Tables S2 and S3). The tasters checked all the available aroma (17) and taste/mouthfeel (12) attributes at least once. Interestingly, tasters did not report any additional descriptors related to occasional over-development, which would be expected since old wines were being tasted. A total of 11 aromas were used for the subsequent correspondence analysis (CA), where 10 were cited at least 10% of the time in one of the white wines and bruised apple was also included because of its relationship with possible oxidation. Therefore, straw, caramel, wet/flint stone and curry were present in white wines but were not included, while spicy and earthy were not used since are characteristic of red and rosé wines. Regarding taste and mouthfeel descriptors, astringency and roughness were not used since they share a low frequency of citation for all wines, as would be expected in white wines (<10%) [35]. Sourness was also cited less than 10% of the time (Table S3) probably because it has an unpleasant connotation and tasters preferred to use the term acidity.
The CA, based on the contingency tables only for white wines, was run using the retained aroma, taste and mouthfeel descriptors (Figure 4). The Chi-square value of the CA was 675 with a high significance in the discrimination (df = 323, p <0.001). The taste and mouthfeel descriptors were not run separately because the p-value was > 0.05, indicating that wine discrimination was mostly due to aroma descriptors. Indeed, CATA might not have had discriminant power regarding taste and mouthfeel properties [36]. The first two CA dimensions explained 76.79% of the variance, which showed an adequate discriminant power elicited by aroma descriptors. The in-mouth perception more distant from the center of the plot was sweetness. This could be explained by the effect of fruity and flowery aromas on the so-called phantom sweetness in dry wines [37].
The localization of the quality scores of the three panels as illustrative variables in the central zone of the plot indicates that quality evaluation did not contribute to the sensory analytic discrimination of the samples.
Proximity in the space cannot be interpreted as direct association between wines [38]. For instance, the closer position of WMo16 than WDa12 to chamomile does not mean that it smells more like chamomile. Consequently, the map is not sufficient to draw a conclusion on the differences between products. The basic correct interpretation is that the farther out a wine lies on an axis, the more frequently that attribute is associated with that wine [38]. Thus, the CA biplot shows a clear distinction of the younger wines (WDo20 and WLi18), in the left part of the quadrant, from the older wines in the opposed quadrant (WDa74, WPe08, WBa91, WPe00 and WDa03). The attributes placed closer to the center mean that are not exclusive of any wine (length, saltiness, bitterness and acidity). The overall sensory projections show that the samples adequately covered the sensory space correspondent to dry white wines of different ages.
To understand the associations between the sensory descriptors, a cluster analysis was run and is depicted in Figure 5. The obtained clusters may be described consistently using a metaphor according to each dominant perception. Indeed, freshness may be used to encompass the descriptors sweetness, fresh fruit, floral, citrus and chamomile, typical of young wines. Another cluster, described as mature, included the aromas of honey and dried fruit linked to body, length and viscosity. A third cluster included only aroma attributes (kerosene, bruised apple, butter, oak and beeswax), gathered under the overall perception of mellowed wines [21]. The sensations of acidity and other in-mouth perceptions (saltiness, bitterness, smoothness and dryness) may be described under the umbrella of austere, mostly used in the popular wine press (e.g., www.wineenthusiast.com/basics/drinks-terms-defined/austere-wine-meaning, accessed on 5 May 2024). The frequency of citation of the descriptors in each sensory cluster is given in Table 4.

3.2.2. Predicted Age

The previous cluster analysis may also be applied to wines, according to their sensory description (Figure 6). The obtained four clusters corresponded to different wines’ ages, as perceived by the tasters. The results show that in 11 wines, the prediction differed by less than 3 years, comprising wines with real ages up to 12.5 years (Table 5). The difference in the prediction was higher for the older wines, being remarkable in the case of WDa74, where the difference was roughly 36 years.
The prediction may be explained by the sensory conceptual space of the wines. Indeed, when wines were grouped according to their predicted age and the frequency of citation of the clusters depicted in Figure 4, the output may be regarded as the sensory evolution of dry white wines during aging (Figure 7). As expected, this evolution was characterised by a decrease in the freshness perception. Yet, some fresh fruitiness may be kept [8], accompanied by an increase in the evolved wine attributes, comprising mature and mellowed perceptual spaces. Interestingly, the austere perception underlies all ages with a slight reduction with time. This behaviour is consistent with the metaphor “mellowed by aging” proposed during the initial conceptual definition [21].

3.3. Overall Factors Affecting Quality Evaluation

The evaluation of the different factors that influence quality was performed by Multiple Factorial Analysis (MFA), using absorbance (420 nm), sensory descriptors and predicted age as variables. The first two components explained 68.7% of the variance (Supplementary Table S4). The variables contributing to the PCA are shown in Figure 8. The axes oppose the perception of freshness (left position) to the perceptions of mature (beeswax and oak) and mellowed (dried fruit, honey and viscosity) wines. In the middle of the plan appears acidity, indicating that this attribute does not distinguishes younger from older wines. Even so, the onset of browning is regarded as a negative event [39]. Figure 7 also shows that quality could be related to an acceptable degree of browning (Abs 420 nm). Therefore, the dry white wines most valorised by the tasters shared sensory attributes typical of older wines, within a predicted age range.
Figure 9 shows the discrimination of the dry white wines based on a priori grou** according to age perception. Wines were clearly separated, with younger samples in the lower left quadrant and mellowed wines in the lower right quadrant, while mature wines were placed in the higher quadrants. Table 6 summarises the quality, sensory and colour characteristics according to the predicted age groups.

3.4. Limitations of the Study

The fact that each cohort had a small number of tasters limited the significance of results regarding the effect of experience on the evaluation of synthetic factors affecting the aesthetic properties of the wines. In addition, the large variability in responses regarding synthetic/aesthetic attributes could probably be reduced by extensive training to calibrate the responses and by outlier deletion. However, no attempt was made to remove outliers since Parr [40] stated that this variability is intrinsic to sensory analysis and “not and error in the machine”. Nevertheless, the sensory analysis using CATA provided information that elicited a consistent definition of the aging sensory concept of dry white wines. The utilization of dark glasses would have reduced the top-down effects on sensory description but the intent was to check if brownish wines could be underscored, which did not occur. In addition, regarding the complex nature of wine ageing it would have been interesting to check the evolution of the sensory attributes with time after pouring wine into the glasses [41].
The experimental outputs obviously depended on the wines selected for the study. Moreover, the valorization of old wines might have been induced by previously informing tasters about the purpose of the research as a deliberate strategy to attract highly reputed professionals. Therefore, even if the tasted samples covered the sensory range from younger to older wines, it would have been useful to have had clearly substandard dark-brown oxidised wines to establish the boundaries between not-faulty/faulty wines, which appear to be easier to define in whites than in red wines [42].
Another limitation concerns the use of other grape varieties, or wines from international regions known for their aging potential [43], to cover sensory features different from those of Portuguese wines.

4. Conclusions and Future Prospects

The results indicate that tasters with different degrees of expertise and experience properly valorised aged dry white wines. The variability in the assessment of properties with aesthetic value indicates that there is an idiosyncratic appreciation that underlies fine-wine quality. In particular, critics and winemakers showed different quality inferences, mainly regarding the contribution of power and balance. To these cohorts, liking was intrinsically related to quality evaluation. The influence of browning on quality evaluation was an indication of top-down processes that were more evident among the critics.
The utilization of CATA methodology enabled us to evidence the attributes consistent with the sensory space of “wines mellowed by age”. The boundaries between younger and older wines were dependent on the decreasing perception of freshness and with the concomitant increase in the perception of maturity and mellowed flavours. A constant perception of austerity spanned the sensory continuum, which may be envisaged as the structure on which the aging potential of dry white wines is built.
Overall, this study demonstrated the fine wine quality attributes of aged dry white wines and contributed to the definition of the sensory space of “wines mellowed by age”. Wine education programs might use this defined sensory space together with the corresponding aesthetic attributes to expand the range of quality perception by consumers.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://mdpi.longhoe.net/article/10.3390/beverages10020044/s1, Figure S1: Tasting sheet used in the study; Table S1: Mean scores of colour (Abs 420 nm) and synthetic/aesthetic attributes given by the 3 tasting cohorts; Table S2: Contingency table of aroma analytical descriptors (those retained for correspondence analysis are written in bold); Table S3: Contingency table of analytical taste and synthetic mouthfeel descriptors (those retained for correspondence analysis are written in bold); Table S4: Eigenvalues and variance of the PCA elicited by the factors affecting quality evaluation.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.M.-F. and M.S.; methodology, M.M.-F. and M.S.; software, M.E. and M.S.; validation, M.M.-F.; investigation, M.E.; writing—original draft preparation, M.E. and M.S.; writing—review and editing, M.M.-F.; supervision, M.M.-F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by national funds from the research unit UID/AGR/04129/2013 (LEAF—Linking, Landscape, Environment, Agriculture and Food Research Center) through Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (Portuguese Foundation for Science and technology—FCT).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author due to the absence of a data repository for this specific dataset.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the wine companies that donated the tasted wines.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Linsenmeier, A.W.; Rauhut, D.; Sponholz, W.R. Ageing and flavour deterioration in wine. In Managing Wine Quality, 2nd ed.; Reynold, A., Ed.; Woodhead Publishing Limited: Cambridge, UK, 2022; pp. 559–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Fischer, U.; Roth, D.; Christmann, M. The impact of geographic origin, vintage and wine estate on sensory properties of Vitis vinifera cv. Riesling wines. Food Qual. Prefer. 1999, 10, 281–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Godden, P.; Francis, L.; Field, J.; Gishen, M.; Coulter, A.; Valente, P.; Hoj, P.; Robinson, E. Wine bottle closures: Physical characteristics and effect on composition and sensory properties of a Semillon wine 1. Performance up to 20 months post-bottling. Aust. J. Grape Wine Res. 2001, 7, 64–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Pons, A.; Lavigne, V.; Thibon, C.; Redon, P.; Loisel, C.; Dubourdieu, D.; Darriet, P. Impact of Closure OTR on the Volatile Compound Composition and Oxidation Aroma Intensity of Sauvignon Blanc Wines during and after 10 Years of Bottle Storage. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2021, 69, 9883–9894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Ballester, J.; Magne, M.; Julien, P.; Noret, L.; Nikolantonaki, M.; Coelho, C.; Gougeon, R.D. Sensory Impact of Polyphenolic Composition on the Oxidative Notes of Chardonnay Wines. Beverages 2018, 4, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Skouroumounis, G.; Kwiatokowsky, M.; Francis, I.; Oakey, Y.; Capone, D.; Duncan, B.; Sefton, M.; Waters, E. The impact of closure type and storage conditions on the composition, colour and flavour properties of a Riesling and a wooded Chardonnay wine during five years’ storage. Aust. J. Grape Wine Res. 2005, 11, 369–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Gabrielli, M.; Fracassetti, D.; Romanini, E.; Colangelo, D.; Tirelli, A.; Lambri, M. Oxygen-induced faults in bottled white wine: A review of technological and chemical characteristics. Food Chem. 2021, 348, 128922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Blackman, J.W.; Hopfer, H.; Saliba, A.J.; Schmidtke, L.M.; Barril, C.; Scollary, G.R. Sensory characterization of Hunter Valley Semillon aged in bottle. Flav. Fragang. J. 2014, 29, 340–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Dacremont, C.; Vickers, Z. Classification of cheeses according to their closeness to the cheddar cheese concept. J. Sens. Stud. 1994, 9, 237–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Candelon, M.; Ballester, J.; Uscidda, N.; Blanquet, J.; Le Fur, Y. Sensory methodology developed for the investigation of Sciaccarello wine concept. OENO One 2004, 38, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ballester, J.; Dacremont, C.; Le Fur, Y.; Etiévant, P. The role of olfaction in the elaboration and use of the Chardonnay wine concept. Food Qual. Prefer. 2005, 16, 351–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Loison, A.; Symoneaux, R.; Deneulin, P.; Thomas-Danguin, T.; Fant, C.; Guérin, L.; Le Fur, Y. Exemplarity measurement and estimation of the level of interjudge agreement for two categories of French red wines. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 40, 240–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Schüttler, A.; Friedel, M.; Jung, R.; Rauhut, D.; Darriet, P. Characterizing aromatic typicality of Riesling wines: Merging volatile compositional and sensory aspects. Food Res. Int. 2015, 69, 26–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Garrido-Bañuelos, G.; Ballester, J.; Buica, A.; Mihnea, M. Exploring the typicality, sensory space, and chemical composition of swedish solaris wines. Foods 2020, 9, 1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kustos, M.; Gambetta, J.; Jeffery, D.; Heymann, H.; Goodman, S.; Bastian, S. A matter of place: Sensory and chemical characterisation of fine Australian Chardonnay and Shiraz wines of provenance. Food Res. Int. 2020, 130, 108903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Sáenz-Navajas, M.P.; Arias-Pérez, I.; Ferrero-del-Teso, S.; Escudero, A.; Ferreira, V.; Fernández-Zurbano, P.; Valentin, D. Access to wine experts’ long-term memory to decipher an ill-defined sensory concept: The case of green red wine. OENO One 2021, 55, 69–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Langlois, J.; Dacremont, C.; Peyron, D.; Valentin, D.; Dubois, D. Lexicon and types of discourse in wine expertise: The case of vin de garde. Food Qual. Prefer. 2011, 22, 491–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Picard, M.; Tempère, S.; de Revel, G.; Marchand, S. A sensory study of the ageing bouquet of red Bordeaux wines: A three-step approach for exploring a complex olfactory concept. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 42, 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Malfeito-Ferreira, M. Fine wine flavour perception and appreciation: Blending neuronal processes, tasting methods and expertise. Trend Food Sci. Technol. 2021, 115, 332–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Barbe, J.; Garbay, J.; Tempère, S. The sensory space of wines: From concept to evaluation and description. A review. Foods 2021, 10, 1424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Sequeira, M.; Malfeito-Ferreira, M. Mental representation of the sensory space of dry white wines “mellowed” by ageing. OENO one 2024. submitted, under the first review round. [Google Scholar]
  22. Ribéreau-Gayon, P.; Glories, Y.; Maujean, A.; Dubourdieu, D. Handbook of Enology. Volume 2: The Chemistry of Wine Stabilization and Treatments, 2nd ed.; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2006; pp. 178–179. [Google Scholar]
  23. ISO 3591:1977; Sensory Analysis—Apparatus—Wine-Tasting Glass. ISO International Standard: Geneva, Switzerland, 1977.
  24. Parr, W.V.; Grose, C.; Hedderley, D.; Maraboli, M.M.; Masters, O.; Araujo, L.D.; Valentin, D. Perception of quality and complexity in wine and their links to varietal typicality: An investigation involving Pinot noir wine and professional tasters. Food Res. Int. 2020, 137, 109423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Ballester, J.; Belascoin, M.; Gonzalez-Hernandez, M.; Parga-Dans, E.; Alonso González, P.; Saenz-Navajas, M.-P. Sensory attributes and quality perception of red natural wines: A comparative study in Spain and France. OENO One 2024, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Shepherd, H.; Parr, W.; Lo Monaco, G.; Heber Rodrigues, H. The meaning of the word elegance as a wine descriptor: Effect of expertise and wine type. Food Res. Int. 2023, 164, 112399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Sackris, D.; Larsen, R. The Perniciousness of Higher-Order Evidence on Aesthetic Appreciation. Dialogue Can Phil. Rev. 2023, 62, 303–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Malfeito-Ferreira, M. Fine wine recognition and appreciation: It is time to change the paradigm of wine tasting. Food Res. Int. 2023, 174, 113668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Morrot, G.; Brochet, F.; Dubourdieu, D. The color of odors. Brain Lang. 2001, 79, 309–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ballester, J.; Abdi, H.; Langlois, J.; Peyron, D.; Valentin, D. The odor of colors: Can wine experts and novices distinguish the odors of white. red. and rosé wines? Chemosens. Percept. 2009, 2, 203–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Wang, Q.J.; Spence, C. Drinking through rosé-coloured glasses: Influence of wine colour on the perception of aroma and flavour in wine experts and novices. Food Res. Int. 2019, 126, 108678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Shepherd, G.M. Neuroenology: How the brain creates the taste of wine. Flavour 2015, 4, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Spence, C. Wine psychology: Basic & applied. Cogn. Res. 2020, 5, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Parr, W.V.; White, K.G.; Heatherbell, D. The nose knows: Influence of colour on perception of wine aroma. J. Wine Res. 2003, 14, 79–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Cheynier, V.; Sarni-Manchado, P. Wine Taste and Mouthfeel. In Managing Wine Quality, 1st ed.; Reynold, A., Ed.; Woodhead Publishing Limited: Cambridge, UK, 2010; pp. 29–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Brand, J.; Panzer, V.; Buica, A. Wine quality drivers: A case study on South African Chenin Blanc and Pinotage wines. Foods 2020, 9, 805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Vitorino, G.; Mota, M.; Malfeito-Ferreira, M. Characterization of sensory perceptions elicited by white wine spiked with different aroma, taste and mouth-feel active molecules. J. Vitic. Enol. 2021, 36, 139–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Peltier, C.; Visalli, M.; Schlich, P. Canonical Variate Analysis of Sensory Profiling Data. J. Sensory Stud. 2015, 30, 316–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Kanavouras, A.; Coutelieris, F.; Karanika, E.; Kotseridis, Y.; Kallithraka, S. Colour change of bottled white wines as a quality indicator. OENO One 2020, 54, 543–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Parr, W.V. Demystifying wine tasting: Cognitive psychology’s contribution. Food Res. Int. 2019, 124, 230–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Wang, Q.J.; Niaura, T.; Kantono, K. How does wine ageing influence perceived complexity? Temporal-Choose-All-That-Apply (TCATA) reveals temporal drivers of complexity in experts and novices. Food Qual. Prefer. 2021, 92, 104230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Franco-Luesma, E.; Honoré-Chedozeau, C.; Ballester, J.; Valentin, D. Oxidation in wine: Does expertise influence the perception? LWT 2019, 116, 108511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Bueno, M.; Culleré, L.; Cacho, J.; Ferreira, V. Chemical and sensory characterisation of oxidative behavior in different wines. Food Res. Int. 2010, 43, 1423–1428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Distribution of quality scores given by critics, oenologists and students (box plots: •, average; horizontal line, median; lower horizontal dash, 10% of the scores; higher horizontal dash, 90% of the scores; box, 25% and 50% of the scores; ♦, extreme scores).
Figure 1. Distribution of quality scores given by critics, oenologists and students (box plots: •, average; horizontal line, median; lower horizontal dash, 10% of the scores; higher horizontal dash, 90% of the scores; box, 25% and 50% of the scores; ♦, extreme scores).
Beverages 10 00044 g001
Figure 2. Correlation between median quality scores of dry white wines among the three panels. ((A) tau-b = 0.033, p = 0.881; (B) tau-b = 0.255, p = 0.152; (C) tau-b = 0.098, p = 0.601).
Figure 2. Correlation between median quality scores of dry white wines among the three panels. ((A) tau-b = 0.033, p = 0.881; (B) tau-b = 0.255, p = 0.152; (C) tau-b = 0.098, p = 0.601).
Beverages 10 00044 g002
Figure 3. Correlation between median quality scores and absorbance of dry white wines among the three panels ((A) critics, tau-b = 0.686, p < 0.001; (B) oenologists, tau-b = 0.438, p = 0.011; (C) students, tau-b = 0.315, p = 0.069).
Figure 3. Correlation between median quality scores and absorbance of dry white wines among the three panels ((A) critics, tau-b = 0.686, p < 0.001; (B) oenologists, tau-b = 0.438, p = 0.011; (C) students, tau-b = 0.315, p = 0.069).
Beverages 10 00044 g003
Figure 4. Correspondence analysis of aroma, taste and mouthfeel descriptors of dry white wines. The median quality scores given by critics, oenologists and students were added as illustrative variables.
Figure 4. Correspondence analysis of aroma, taste and mouthfeel descriptors of dry white wines. The median quality scores given by critics, oenologists and students were added as illustrative variables.
Beverages 10 00044 g004
Figure 5. Cluster dendrogram of sensory descriptors elicited by dry white wines of different ages.
Figure 5. Cluster dendrogram of sensory descriptors elicited by dry white wines of different ages.
Beverages 10 00044 g005
Figure 6. Cluster dendrogram grou** dry white wines of different ages according to their sensory description.
Figure 6. Cluster dendrogram grou** dry white wines of different ages according to their sensory description.
Beverages 10 00044 g006
Figure 7. Sensory characterisation of dry white wines grouped according to their predicted age.
Figure 7. Sensory characterisation of dry white wines grouped according to their predicted age.
Beverages 10 00044 g007
Figure 8. Multiple Factorial Analysis of dry white wines displaying the quantitative variables related to colour (Abs 420 nm), predicted age, sensory attributes and quality.
Figure 8. Multiple Factorial Analysis of dry white wines displaying the quantitative variables related to colour (Abs 420 nm), predicted age, sensory attributes and quality.
Beverages 10 00044 g008
Figure 9. Multiple Factorial Analysis of dry white wines grouped according to the age perception.
Figure 9. Multiple Factorial Analysis of dry white wines grouped according to the age perception.
Beverages 10 00044 g009
Table 1. Origin and absorbance (420 nm) of the tasted wines.
Table 1. Origin and absorbance (420 nm) of the tasted wines.
CodeBrandCompanyRegion (Sub-Region)YearAbsorbance
WDa74Branco Dão bCEV a NelasDão19740.201
WBa91Vinhas Velhas bLuís PatoBairrada19910.782
WPe00Catarina b,cBacalhôaPenínsula de Setúbal20000.484
WDa03Casa de Santar bSociedade Agrícola de SantarDão20030.286
WBe03Quinta dos Termos bQuinta dos TermosBeira Interior20030.284
WPe08Pasmados b,cJosé Maria da FonsecaPenínsula de Setúbal20080.695
WAl10Cartuxa bFundação Eugénio de AlmeidaAlentejo20100.221
WAl11Dom Rafael bHerdade do MouchãoAlentejo20110.266
WBe11Quinta Vale do Ruivo (Arinto) dJosé Madeira AfonsoBeira Interior20110.111
WMe11Alvarinho (Alvarinho) dQuinta do SoalheiroVerde (Melgaço).20110.379
WDa12Pedra Cancela bLusoviniDão20120.172
WMo12Alvarinho (Alvarinho) dPalácio da BrejoeiraVerde (Monção)20120.181
WMe12Alvarinho (Alvarinho) dQuinta do SoalheiroVerde (Melgaço)20120.322
WDo13Mau Feitio bVinilourençoDouro20130.197
WAl15Sericaia bAres AlentejanosAlentejo20150.143
WMo16Alvarinho (Alvarinho) dPalácio da BrejoeiraVerde (Monção)20160.151
WLi18Quinta do Monte d’Oiro (Viognier) c,dQuinta do Monte D’OiroLisboa20180.092
WDo20Fraga da Galhofa (Moscatel Galego) dVinilourençoDouro20200.091
Ro06Fraga da Galhofa b (Rosé)VinilourençoDouro20060.591
Re74Periquita (Red) (Castelão) dJosé Maria da FonsecaPenínsula de Setúbal19744.939
a State wine research center in Nelas, Dão region. b Blended wines with local grape varieties. c Barrique aging on lees. d Grape variety.
Table 2. Chi-square (χ2) values of the scores given to synthetic and aesthetic attributes of all wines by the tasting panels (df, degrees of freedom; significant p-values < 0.05).
Table 2. Chi-square (χ2) values of the scores given to synthetic and aesthetic attributes of all wines by the tasting panels (df, degrees of freedom; significant p-values < 0.05).
Attributesχ2dfp
Familiarity11.9200.921
Number of flavours15.4200.751
Ease of identification10.6200.956
Harmonious19.8200.471
Balance26.4200.152
Linger22.1200.333
Strong/powerful24.1200.238
Complexity27.0200.134
Liking29.6200.076
Quality26.1200.161
Table 3. Correlation (r) between the median quality scores and the rest of the synthetic descriptors and liking. Bold numbers indicate strong correlations using Kendall’s tau-b (>0.600).
Table 3. Correlation (r) between the median quality scores and the rest of the synthetic descriptors and liking. Bold numbers indicate strong correlations using Kendall’s tau-b (>0.600).
Tasting
Panels
FamiliarityNumber of FlavoursEase of IdentificationHarmoniousBalanceLingerStrong and PowerfulComplexityLiking
Overall0.193 10.424 30.1500.467 30.580 30.631 30.575 30.662 30.770 3
Critics0.2110.474 20.2060.358 10.505 20.691 30.600 30.758 30.874 3
Oenologists0.0630.600 30.2630.589 30.705 30.674 30.484 20.726 30.842 3
Students0.0260.201−0.0580.406 20.480 20.1850.1800.522 20.522 2
1 p < 0.05, 2 p < 0.01, 3 p < 0.001.
Table 4. Frequency of citation of the perceptual overall clusters.
Table 4. Frequency of citation of the perceptual overall clusters.
ClustersDescriptorsPredicted Age (Years)
<23–74–107–16
FreshnessFresh fruit231591
Floral2212125
Citrus1818127
Sweetness12783
Chamomile13884
Total88605020
MatureKerosene1558
Bruised apple35510
Butter3358
Oak14711
Beeswax25812
Total10222849
MellowedBody771514
Length9111413
Viscosity491212
Honey261117
Dried fruit271116
Total24406272
AustereSaltiness10111211
Bitterness9101012
Smoothness1510129
Dryness14151316
Acidity27262323
Total75717172
Table 5. Average age prediction of the white wines reported by the tasting cohorts.
Table 5. Average age prediction of the white wines reported by the tasting cohorts.
Cluster aWineReal AgePredicted Age aDifference
Very youngWDo200.51.4−0.9
WLi182.52.10.4
YoungWDo137.56.51.0
WAl155.53.42.1
WBe119.53.75.8
MatureWMe128.59.5−1.0
WMo164.53.80.7
WAl119.58.31.2
WMe119.57.22.3
WDa128.55.03.5
WBe0317.57.010.5
MellowedWAl1010.510.7−0.2
WPe0812.513.8−1.3
WMo128.57.31.2
WPe0020.513.66.9
WDa0318.510.97.6
WBa9129.516.313.2
WDa7446.510.935.6
a Mean value of the predicted age given by each member of the tasting panel.
Table 6. Range of variation in the quality, sensory and colour characteristics according to predicted age wine groups a.
Table 6. Range of variation in the quality, sensory and colour characteristics according to predicted age wine groups a.
CharacteristicsSensory DescriptorsVery YoungYoungMatureMellowed
Mean predicted age (years)-1.4–2.13.4–6.53.8–9.57.3–16.3
Median quality scores-4.32–4.494.33–5.274.80–6.174.93–6.33
Mean colour (Abs 420 nm)-0.091–0.0920.111–0.1970.151–0.3790.181–0.782
Citation frequencyFresh fruit22–2310–144–170–4
Citrus16–2011–199–174–13
Floral21–2310–1310–152–11
Chamomile13–136–115–92–9
Bruised apple3–35–82–73–11
Butter1–42–33–63–10
Beewax0–35–63–108–18
Honey0–32–124–1413–24
Dried fruit0–34–96–1312–19
Kerosene0–21–102–75–13
Oak0–24–61–128–14
Acidity26–2723–3022–2616–29
Smoothness15–157–119–184–14
Dryness11–1613–1911–1412–19
Sweetness8–163–74–140–6
Bitterness6–129–128–159–13
Length8–109–1113–189–18
Body6–76–128–177–18
Saltiness2–66–88–1310–14
Viscosity2–66–810–178–17
a Wines listed in Table 5 and values used in the Multiple Factorial Analysis depicted in Figure 7 and Figure 8.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Esteves, M.; Sequeira, M.; Malfeito-Ferreira, M. Definition of the Sensory and Aesthetic Spaces of Dry White Wines with Aging Ability by Experienced Tasters. Beverages 2024, 10, 44. https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages10020044

AMA Style

Esteves M, Sequeira M, Malfeito-Ferreira M. Definition of the Sensory and Aesthetic Spaces of Dry White Wines with Aging Ability by Experienced Tasters. Beverages. 2024; 10(2):44. https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages10020044

Chicago/Turabian Style

Esteves, Marta, Mariana Sequeira, and Manuel Malfeito-Ferreira. 2024. "Definition of the Sensory and Aesthetic Spaces of Dry White Wines with Aging Ability by Experienced Tasters" Beverages 10, no. 2: 44. https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages10020044

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop